Category Archives: Philosophy

And the fork ran away with the spoon

In an earlier blog post, I rambled on incoherently about the idea of conceptual synesthesia. This is the idea that certain people assign colors and gender and even personalities to letters and numbers. This recent article about language makes the point that gender assignment for objects is often integral to languages, and different languages assign different genders to the same object.

In the 1990s, for example, psychologists compared associations between speakers of German and Spanish. There are many inanimate nouns whose genders in the two languages are reversed. A German bridge is feminine (die Brücke), for instance, but el puente is masculine in Spanish; and the same goes for clocks, apartments, forks, newspapers, pockets, shoulders, stamps, tickets, violins, the sun, the world and love. On the other hand, an apple is masculine for Germans but feminine in Spanish, and so are chairs, brooms, butterflies, keys, mountains, stars, tables, wars, rain and garbage. When speakers were asked to grade various objects on a range of characteristics, Spanish speakers deemed bridges, clocks and violins to have more “manly properties” like strength, but Germans tended to think of them as more slender or elegant. With objects like mountains or chairs, which are “he” in German but “she” in Spanish, the effect was reversed.

In a different experiment, French and Spanish speakers were asked to assign human voices to various objects in a cartoon. When French speakers saw a picture of a fork (la fourchette), most of them wanted it to speak in a woman’s voice, but Spanish speakers, for whom el tenedor is masculine, preferred a gravelly male voice for it.

Writing advice

I’ve mentioned that I’m currently working on a horror/suspense novel. I’m about 12,000 words in, and it’s coming along, but I’m realizing that the text I’m producing is going to need some work once the whole thing is completed. You see, as I write, if I find myself stuck searching for a certain word or phrase, I tend to just put down something to get the point across, with the intent of going back later and really finessing things. I’m starting to understand that after I’ve got everything down on the page, I’m going to need to go through the book paragraph by paragraph, or even sentence by sentence, and apply that finishing coat of paint. Thinking about this topic today led to a breakthrough in my understanding of the process of writing a novel.

Before I get into that, let me ask you question. Do you know how sometimes you go out at night and you pick up a prostitute and she angers you by reminding you of your mother, so you kill her and stuff her in a meat locker in your basement? And, every couple weeks or so, you go down there and open the locker, and see that she’s decayed just a bit more, until finally she’s just a bunch of bones? Well, now imagine if you could flip some kind of cosmic switch and cause time to happen in reverse. In this case, you would start out with a pile of dusty bones and slowly you would see flesh appear, followed by skin, and ultimately a perfectly normal dead prostitute. Well, I believe writing a novel is like watching a dead prostitute decay if time went in reverse.

Need me to clarify? Well, when you start our writing a novel, you have a basic concept of the story. I had a 20 page screenplay treatment. That’s the equivalent of the skeleton. Then you get in there and think through the detail that more, adding plot points and descriptive detail. This is like putting meat on the bones of the dead prostitute skeleton in your meat locker. Then you go in and take a pass at actually writing the novel, getting down to the nitty-gritty of sentences and paragraphs. This is comparable to having the body of a dead prostitute without skin. The final step, is of course applying that finishing touch — massaging the text so that the descriptions and metaphors make the point without being too cliché, finding the appropriate word where needed. This is the process of putting the skin back on the body. It seems minor — skin is a very small part of the human body — but it’s important. You can take a very attractive woman like Paris Hilton, but if you remove all her skin, she’s not so attractive anymore, is she?

So that’s my theory. A lot of authors will compare the process of writing a novel to giving birth to a child. That’s a legitimate comparison, but I don’t think it’s fully accurate. I think it’s more correct to say that writing a novel is a lot like watching the body of a dead prostitute reverse-decay after you flip a cosmic switch which causes time to go backwards.

I hope that’s helpful to any budding writers out there.

Solving the housing crisis

In the past, I’ve proffered my theory of the digital vagabond — an individual who only owns what he can maintain in digital form on an iPad type device. This fellow, travels the world freely with no need for a house or excess goods.

As the spiraling housing market continues to destroy America’s fortunes, I return to this theory. Why is the house with a white picket fence the epitome of the American ideal? Is slaving away to earn yourself a measurable amount of square feet the best use of your time? Libertarian writer Will Wilkinson is often contemplating such questions, usually in an effort to decry housing subsidies. Here’s an example:

…American culture really does relentlessly assault Americans with the American idea that owning an American house is an essential American part of the best and most authentic American way of American living.

So then, how would the digital vagabond live? Well, we’ve all heard of these Japanese hotel cubicles. If you take a look here, you see they’re not that bad. I would compare them to the berths available on your standard sailboat. Plenty of room to move around, and even equipped with a television.

I’m not saying this sort of lifestyle would be for everyone. I understand there’s a lot of sniveling wretches who demand their own poolroom and garage workshop. And, if they’re fine sacrificing 40 years of their life to get that*, more power to the idiots. But I think it is quite conceivable that someone could live a life without a house, and still be quite comfortable and happy. And I think we should look at how we live now — in apartments or houses that keep us generally routed to one area — and reconsider our reasons for doing so.

* Of course, the real reason your average guy decides that buying a house is important is because he realizes he needs to prove his ability to provide for whatever woman he’s hooked up with and the children they have sired. This offers still more evidence for my theory that women are the root of all the world’s problems.

Rethinking work

Occasionally, I make note of interesting ideas and concepts that were historically believed to be true and are now thought of as absurd. For instance, just today I was reading that Aristotle thought the urge to sleep was brought on in the human body by the stomach’s digestion process causing heat to rise (don’t ask me to explain that one.) The fact that women were thought unfit to vote until fairly recently might stand as another example. (I’m resisting the innumerable jokes that could be made at this point.)

This forces an obvious consideration. What modern beliefs will be shown to be absurd in the light of the future? As I read more about the brain and its functions, I begin to suspect that the eight hour workday will fall into the dustbin of history. I’m not saying the idea of working eight hours a day will become outdated, but rather the idea of working eight consecutive hours. Your brain goes through various states during the day, some are very conducive to deep thinking, some are better for passive creativity, some are pretty much worthless for anything. The idea that you’re going to get eight quality hours out of a person in a consecutive period of time seems unlikely. It would make more sense to figure out the periods during the day a person is best suited for a particular type of work, and assign the appropriate work to the appropriate period. It also seems very likely that having people take a nap somewhere after lunchtime would increase their productivity.

Of course, people don’t live simply for their jobs. They need to have mental brainpower available for the challenges of family life, and their own hobbies and interests. This is why I propose we eliminate the modern family from the equation, and instead provide workers with completely servile but incredibly realistic sexual robots. We can also use brain drugs to ensure that people do not pursue activities outside of work, and exist solely to serve the corporate oligarchy. Only then can we truly be happy.

Can we have free will with no soul?

One of the great debates in human philosophy, as well as the subject of a terrific Rush song, is the question of free will? Do we control our actions? Obviously, to some degree, the answer is no. We don’t choose to breathe or blink or to reflexively jerk our leg when a doctor hits our knee with a hammer. But for higher actions — treating people around us with respect, not killing people who annoy us — we tend to give credit to our free will. We presume these are choices we are making. Additionally, the legal systems of every society man has ever known presume people bear some responsibility for their actions.

Damage to our brains can limit the range of choices from which we can freely choose. If the part of your brain that controls action (the motor cortex) is damaged via a stroke, you may want to move your arm, you may choose to move your arm, but that arm ain’t moving. On a more subtle level, damage to the prefrontal cortex can limit our ability to do long-term planning. The result is that our decisions become more childlike, only focused on the here and now. Damage to the amygdala — heavily involved in emotion — can produce psychopathic behavior and an inability to empathize.

This would seem to imply that we don’t have free will, that our decisions are entirely based on brain physiology. By this argument, our brains are much like a computer: data is inputted (we observe our annoying neighbor asking to borrow our lawnmower), calculations are performed on the data (we determined we’ve had it up to here with his crap), and the result is spit out (brain orders fist to punch him in the face).

But what if we think of these components of the brain as not so much functions, but tools? Let me return to the computer analogy. An Internet browser running on a computer can open up a webpage and render it in an organized fashion, converting digital information into text and pictures. But the computer can’t decide it wants to go to a particular webpage (say, a quality, informative and amusing website like acidlogic.com.) You need to do that. In that sense, the computer is a tool you use to access content. So what if we think of parts of the brain — the prefrontal cortex, the motor cortex, amygdala etc. — as tools that can be used to work towards certain goals? Then the question becomes, who is using these tools, who is utilizing the functionality of the brain from the outside? The answer would have to be the soul. (Well, may be the answer doesn’t have to be the soul, but for the purposes of this blog post it does.)

I have numerous problems with this theory. For the most part, I don’t believe in souls, and I’m not even clear what they’re supposed to be. (For the most part, “soul” seems to be a term encompassing all the aspects about human beings, brains and behavior that we don’t understand.) Would the soul be our “consciousness” — whatever that is? And what happens to the soul when part of the brain is damaged? If your amygdala goes bad, does your soul also lose the ability to emphasize?

This is all very vague and fuzzyheaded, I know. But it does seem to me that, in order for there to be free will, there would have to be some additional component aside from the brain itself doing things. If we only have the brain — which clearly seems to operate in a very deterministic fashion — we cannot have free will.

(The concept of free will has taken another hit from the work of neurologist Benjamin Libit. You can read the specifics at this link, but basically, it seems like ideas for actions “bubble up” from the brain to the consciousness, not the other way around. Our consciousness is taking orders from the brain.)

Revulsion

One interesting science nugget I think we’ve all heard is the idea that certain phobias are programmed into our DNA. For example, we’re supposed to have a revulsion of snakes and spiders since they are notorious carriers of venom. Personally, I don’t have any real issues with spiders, but I can speak to an interesting experience I recently had with snakes: I was walking up a mountain near my dad’s house and came across a large rattlesnake laying out in the sun, a recent meal making a noticeable bulge in his belly. It did give me a little bit of the heebie-jeebies. I could feel a jolt run up and down my spine, and a slight turning in my stomach. It’s weird, because I think this is specific to rattlesnakes. I spent a lot of my summers as a kid in the Rocky Mountains of Montana, and I used to catch snakes all the time with no ill effects. But this was the first time I’d seen a rattlesnake.

Another likely example of a pre-programmed phobia? Our revulsion towards dead, rotting, decaying corpses. This works to our benefit because corpses often contain infectious agents — that’s why we bury or burn our dead.

Incest is another example of something we seem to have an innate revulsion for. Consider the following thought experiment:

Julie and Mark are brother and sister. They are traveling together in France on summer vacation from college. One night they are staying alone in a cabin near the beach. They decide that it would be interesting and fun if they tried making love. At very least it would be a new experience for each of them. Julie was already taking birth control pills, but Mark uses a condom too, just to be safe. They both enjoy making love, but they decide not to do it again. They keep that night as a special secret, which makes them feel even closer to each other. What do you think about that, was it OK for them to make love? — A thought experiment devised by Jonathan Heidt

Some might argue that there are rational (not innate) reasons to oppose such an act. Cognitive psychologist Steven Pinker disagrees.

Most people immediately declare that these acts are wrong and then grope to justify why they are wrong. It’s not so easy. In the case of Julie and Mark, people raise the possibility of children with birth defects, but they are reminded that the couple were diligent about contraception. They suggest that the siblings will be emotionally hurt, but the story makes it clear that they weren’t. They submit that the act would offend the community, but then recall that it was kept a secret. Eventually many people admit, “I don’t know, I can’t explain it, I just know it’s wrong.” — Steven Pinker

“I can’t explain it,” would seem to be an indicator of some innate revulsion, either built into our DNA, or planted early in our conscious existence.

I’ve long thought the same might be true for homophobia. Certainly, from the point of view of a species looking to continue its existence, homosexuality would be a detriment. It might not be a problem if homosexuals are a small percentage of the species population, but, obviously, the more homosexuals a species has, the less likely its odds of long-term survival. (There are a number of interesting, theoretical caveats to this statement, but I think the gist of it stands.) As such, it’s possible that a revulsion to homosexual acts could be built into our DNA.

Here’s the interesting thing: I find I don’t really feel these revulsions (aside from the rattlesnake thing.) Rotting, decaying corpses? Boring. The incestuous relationship described in the experiment above? Doesn’t really bug me. General homosexual behavior? Eh.

Here’s an interesting thought experiment to test your own level of revulsion. Imagine you are performing oral sex on a person of your gender. I mean really imagine this — contemplate the groans the person is making, the taste and texture of their sex in or around your mouth. Then, look up at them and realize that they are a long dead, rotting zombie. And as you watch, hundreds of snakes and spiders crawl out of their eye and nose holes.

You can let me know your results in the comments.

Rethinking cannibalism

There’s a recent issue of Time magazine with a cover story entitled “What Animals Think” which discusses the current state of science in regards to measuring how aware and self-conscious animals are about themselves and others. It’s fascinating stuff, much of it centered around a 29-year-old bonobo ape who has a very rich and complex vocabulary (not just nouns and verbs.)

There’s a moral aspect to the article as well. Obviously, if animals are smarter than we think, that perhaps we should rethink killing, torturing (which is what much of animal research is) and eating them. The animal rights ethicist Pete Singer is quoted; he basically argues that you should only eat animals that are devoid of consciousness, such as oysters and mussels. It strikes me that there’s perhaps another tact one can take here. For years, we’ve been allowing ourselves to eat animals based on the assumption that they are not like us. If science is slowly revealing that they are like us, perhaps we should then allow ourselves to eat each other. Certainly, cannibalism would be helpful in dealing with many of the issues facing humanity: overpopulation, starvation, obesity, global warming.

Can a moral case be made for cannibalism? I think it’s a question science needs to take a long hard look at.

The fallibility of thought balloons

Anyone who’s into fine art and high culture is, of course, aware of the modern comic book. And thus you know that whenever a character in a comic book thinks thoughts they are enclosed in an little “thought bubble.” Within these thought bubbles, a character might think something like, “When Doc Ock and Kraven the Hunter kidnapped Mary Jane,I was certain that the game was up. I never could have predicted that the Captain Universe powers would reappear!”

There’s one big problem with the thought bubble concept: people don’t think with an inner dialogue. Our “inner life” is not so much a consistent narrative as it is an ongoing flicker of thoughts and sensations. In the above example, Spiderman wouldn’t mentally speak those thoughts; rather he would have a sensation of Doctor Octopus and Kraven the Hunter and then a reflection of the danger that they had placed Mary Jane in. This might be accompanied by mild visceral sensations — a quickening of the pulse and tightening of the stomach when he thinks of almost losing his beloved — as well as the strange tangential left turns the brain can often take; he might be thinking about the tiger-like Kraven the Hunter, and the process would guide his mind towards the notion of felines, which might make him briefly think about his neighbor’s cat.

A book I’ve been reading — Jonah Lehrer’s “Proust Was a Neuroscientist” — has an interesting chapter on how the writing of Virginia Woolf tackled the challenge of trying to capture our inner life in words. Woolf, who spent so much time analyzing her own mental state that she eventually killed herself, was searching for the “self.” At any given moment, say, a dinner party, we are assaulted with numerous perceptions. The smell of food, the taste of the food, the appearance of our surroundings, the innumerable conversations occurring around us etc. But our self only focuses on some of these perceptions. Lehrer describes how Woolf wrestled with this issue.

But how does the self a rise? How do we continually emerge from our sensations, from the “scraps, orts and fragments” of which the mind is made?

Woolf realized that the self emerges via the act of attention. We bind together our sensory parts by experiencing them from a particular point of view. During this process, some sensations are ignored, while others are highlighted.

Woolf, Lehrer argues, eventually realized her search for the self was fruitless.

How does the self transcend the separateness of its attentive moments? How does a process become us? For Woolf, the answer was simple: the self is an illusion.… just as a novelist creates a narrative, a person creates a sense of being. The self is simply our work of art, a fiction created by the brain in order to make sense of its own disunity… If it didn’t exist, then nothing would exist. We would be a brain full of characters, hopelessly searching for author.”

In a sense, Woolf was arguing that the contents of our inner “thought balloon” is our self. While it isn’t literally verbally narrating our thoughts — comic book style — it is the story that ties all our jumbled perceptions and sensations into a logical narrative. And of course, if you lose that sense of self, as people sometimes do, the results are not pretty.

Did I just totally blow your mind?

One other thing I realized while reading this chapter on Virginia Woolf: she was a total babe, reminiscent of Winona Ryder. Here’s a pic.

Death with dignity

There’s an article in a recent New Yorker about the various medical dilemmas the medical establishment faces when treating a person at the end of their life. Do you do everything you can to keep them alive for as long as possible, replacing their biological functionality with machines? Or can you try and grant the patient some dignity by letting them die at home, without extraordinary measures? Or can you, as the article insinuates, try to meld both approaches?

My feeling is that most death, whether prolonged or not, is undignified and often quite painful. Whether you die because the machine that’s been pumping air into your lungs can’t keep up with your other failing organs or a more natural death — essentially starvation or suffocation — it’s going to suck. That’s why I’ve always advocated for out and out euthanasia. People should have some mechanical monitor placed in their head that keeps tabs on their mental function, pain levels and muscular ability. When this drops below a certain level, a device placed in the brainstem should activate, causing a deluge of microwaves to fire and instantly cook the brain.

There’s only one downside to this: as we know from watching the remake of “The Last House on the Left,” microwaving a person’s head causes it to explode. So it is possible a person could be having a sensitive conversation with their niece about the meaning of life, only to have their head and brain matter splatter all over the family. It is also possible that if a family dog or cat is there, this pet might immediately start eating the tasty fried brains now splattered all over the place. But this is only a small downside to what I feel is a otherwise quite elegant solution to an issue that has plagued mankind since the beginning.

Hippies in Paradise: the Kerista Commune

Post-reading “Sex at Dawn” I found myself browsing through the web for information on nonstandard sexual relationships. I came across discussion of “polyfidelity” which is essentially group marriage. This led me to a website for the “Kerista Commune,” a polyfidelous experiment that took place in San Francisco (where else?) from the early 70s to the early 90s.

The website, maintained by ex-members of the commune, is fascinating and includes a pretty honest assessment of the whole project. On one hand, the advantages of living in such a society are unpretentiously listed.

…once you were in Kerista, you had a real sense of belonging to a tribe. You always had a group of friends to move around with. You didn’t have to worry about paying your bills – the community took care of that. Those of us who were parents did not have to bear the stress and strain of raising kids alone. You always had help figuring out a difficult problem. And, somewhat more abstract, yet still significant, you always had a sense or belief that you were doing something good for the world.

But there’s also much discussion of the downsides.

Our living spaces were disgustingly messy and unaesthetic, largely because no one felt any personal responsibility for them. It was everyone’s – and therefore, no one’s – problem. People felt free to spend money on all kinds of things in a way that they would never do if they were solely responsible for balancing their chequebooks and making ends meet. (And, as it turns out, when the accounting was done after the commune’s demise, we found that our communal fund had been running in the red for years.)

Every ex-Keristan I have talked with remembers numerous instances of going along with the prevailing group sentiment on an issue rather than take a contrary stand, or, worse still, without even bothering to really think the issue through independently… There are memories of this sort about which many of us will probably continue to cringe for years to come . . . times we gave some innocent person a hard time for thinking, saying, or doing something that didn’t synch with current Keristan doctrine.

Though we had never considered ourselves to be guru-centred (after all, we believed in democracy, equality of the sexes, and other “politically correct” positions), from our perspective today, Kerista was in many respects a cult with a charismatic leader. Jud’s forceful personal style of conversation and confrontation became the model for how Keristans related to each other and outsiders; only the most courageous Keristans dared to openly disagree with Jud.

We used to scoff at people who would show up at one of our rap groups and ask, “But what if you end up in a group with someone you aren’t attracted to?” We would tell them they obviously didn’t understand-you only joined a group if you wanted to be with all the people in it, and they all, wanted to be with you. That was the ideal, which made sense. In reality it was often not that way. Many of us did find ourselves at different times in bed with people that, on our own, there was no way on earth we’d have ended up with. The way it sometimes worked was that a few influential members of a group would be interested in a new person, and they would “gestalt” (read, harangue) others who didn’t share that feeling until they assented to accept the new person.

So how exactly did they arrange these polyfedilous relationships? There was a schedule which mandated whom would be sleeping with whom each night. In strict egalitarian fashion, after a person slept with a partner, they would sleep with every other potential partner before returning to that first partner*.

Did being paired with a person mandate sex?

Sex was not mandatory, but it was expected. Most people did have sex, as far as I know. If you weren’t having sex with a certain someone then something was probably wrong in your dyad. Rotating partners every night is a sure way to feel like you should fuck this person who you hadn’t been with in several days.

The whole website appears to be an interesting overview of the appeal and follies of hippie culture.

* The modern classical composer Arnold Schonberg had a similar system devised for music. He would create orders of musical notes containing all the notes in the chromatic scale, and then dictate that his compositions had to use every other note in the order, before reusing the first note.